Pollinators get a buzz off caffeine, too

Most plants can reproduce asexually – that is, new plant life is created without the need for female and male sex cells to combine. However, amongst plants there is a variety of ways in which they can reproduce sexually. Sexual reproduction confers certain advantages to organisms: for example, by the remixing of sexual cells, more variation is maintained within a population and there are more likely to be some organisms able to withstand environmental changes. Animal-mediated pollination is a common way by which plants distribute their sex cells to other individuals in a population. Plant have evolved a wide variety of mechanisms to increase the likelihood of pollination, including conspicuous flowers, enticing scents, and sweet nectar rewards. Coffea and Citrus are plant genera which both exhibit attractive flowers and produce more fruit and seeds when pollinated by bees (Ricketts, 2004), which they reward with nectar. However, the nectar of species in these genera contain another, more harmful substance.
© Geraldine Wright

Species of Coffea and Citrus contain caffeine in their nectar, as well as sugars. Amongst our own species, more caffeine is consumed than any other psychoactive drug (Nehlig, 1999). Despite the stimulating effect this substance induces in people, in nature caffeine is nor usually a treat but a threat: high doses generally have a defensive purpose for plants and produce a negative effect in animals. Why, then, would a pollinator reward contain caffeine – surely it would be disadvantageous to the plant to cause serious harm to its pollinators and would thus be selected against? Wright et al. (2013) proposed to answer this question.

© 7bigspoons.com
Bees are not impervious to the toxic effects of caffeine (Nehlig, 1999). However, in low doses, they do not harm the insects. Wright et al. (2013) decided to investigate how low doses of caffeine affect bees and whether they incur any advantage to the plants. Given caffeine’s enhancing learning and memory effects in humans, Wright et al. (2013) asked whether these qualities were improved in bees. One may asked how a bee’s learning and memory capabilities can be appreciated. To do so, Wright et al. (2013) trained their bees to associate a scent with reward. They exposed bees to a particular scent for a few seconds and rewarded them with a sugary solution with varying amounts of caffeine. This way, all bees had the opportunity to associate the smell to the reward, but different bees received different amounts of caffeine. After 24 hours, the bees were exposed to the scent again, and then again after 72 hours. Although there seemed to be very little learning enhancement, bees that had been administered low doses of caffeine were three times likelier to recognise the scent they associated with reward – which is appreciable by the extension of their mouthparts, as if they were to eat –, than a control group which had not received any caffeine in their sugary solution. After 72 hours, twice as many of the previously-caffeneited bees remembered the scent than did bees that did not receive the drug. In a natural system, it is good news for the plant that the pollinator’s memory of its rewarding scent is remembered.

As is the way in science, these interesting results prompted more questions. Wright et al. (2013) wanted to determine by what mechanisms caffeine affects the insects’ memory capabilities. The authors imaged the electrical activity in intact bees’ brains, specifically looking at the cells associated with learning and memory. They found that, in a similar way to how the substance acts in our own brain cells, caffeine excites nerve pulses in these cells.

Thus, despite the damaging consequences of high doses of caffeine, Wright et al. (2013) demonstrate how producing mildly caffeinated nectar confers a selective advantage to plants by enhancing the memory of pollinators. Additionally, they have shown how the neuronal responses to caffeine in bees are not dissimilar to those in – the very different – human brain.


Cited literature:

Nehlig, A. (1999) Are we dependent upon coffee and caffeine? A review on human and animal data. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 23(4), 563-576.

Ricketts, T. H. (2004) Tropical Forest Fragments Enhance Pollinator Activity in Nearby Coffee Crops. Conservation Biology, 18(5), 1262-1271.

Wright, G. A., Baker, D. D., Palmer, M. J., Stabler, D., Mustard, J. A., Power, E. F., Borland, A. M. & Stevenson, P. C. (2013) Caffeine in Floral Nectar Enhances a Pollinator's Memory of Reward. Science, 339(6124),1202-1204.

Comments

People read...

Happy World Tapir Day!

Twelve and a half months in Ecuador

Hips Don't Lie #1 - Hip hip, hurray!